代写 会员中心 TAG标签
网站地图 RSS
英国essay澳洲essay 美国essay 加拿大essay MBA Essay Essay格式范文
返回首页
当前位置: 写作值吧 > ESSAY > 澳洲essay >

澳洲留学课程essay代写

时间:2014-08-07 13:20来源:www.szdhsjt.com 作者:felicia 点击:
本文是一篇澳洲留学课程essay代写服务。在澳大利亚,大约有9400个铁路平交道口,其中2650个(30%)是活跃的过境点,剩下的6060都是被动交叉口(马修斯和福特,2002)。如果处理不当就会造成巨大的人员伤

素颜格格 三少,工作流 i8小时,复仇军囚犯标识牌

铁路平交路口的类型

在澳大利亚,大约有9400个铁路平交道口,其中2650个(30%)是活跃的过境点,剩下的6060都是被动交叉口(马修斯和福特,2002)。除此之外还有职业的和私人的铁路平交道口。“活跃”平交路口指的是主动保护的平交路口,比如信号和/或水栅门,当列车经过的时候能够自动操作。“被动”平交路口指的是只有信号和/或交通标志的十字路口。在澳大利亚,服务的车辆大约有2400辆(ARRB运输研究,2002)。
 

问题

事故案例

很明显,铁路交通十字路口的行人受到的影响最大,其次是机动车辆(澳大利亚,2002)。这也进一步解释了独特的撞车事件,主要交通干线上的客运列车相撞可能导致生命和财产的巨大损失。
 

在过去的几年里,乘客需求不断增加,列车的速度,隔音,列车安静度,卡车的大小,货运的需求,预计会增加事故的严重程度。这些项目的变化使其更难以减少这些事故的发生及其影响。
 

REVIEW OF LEVEL CROSSING SAFETY

Types of Railway Level Crossing

In Australia, there are about 9,400 railway level crossings, of which 2,650 (30%) are active crossings and remaining 6,060 are passive crossings (Ford and Matthews et al., 2002). In addition to that there are occupational, cane and private railway level crossings. 'Active' level crossings are the crossings which active protection such as signals and/or boom gates which operate automatically when a train is approaching. 'Passive' level crossings are the crossings which have only signs and/or pavement markings. In Australia there are about 2,400 locomotives in service (ARRB Transport Research et al., 2002).
 

Issues

Cases of crashes

It's clear that the pedestrians are worst effected at the railway level crossings followed by motor vehicles (ATSB et al., 2002). This also explains distinctive crash events, but a major crash involving a bus or a passenger train could result in considerable loss of life and property.
 

For the past years, the increase in passenger demand, speed of trains, sound proofing, train quietness, size of trucks, freight demand, were expected to increase the severity of crashes. These changes in the courses make it more difficult to reduce these accidents and their effects.
 

Pedestrian Cases

It is known that 60% of the reported deaths at RLCs are the pedestrians but there is no immediate information available readily on a national basis. Therefore at this stage it is little bit difficult to justify improvements on safety, except on a case by case basis. And it becomes critical when disabled persons are involved For example, in Queensland, these safety improvements are generally undertaken in a system wide strategic approach.
 

Vulnerable Cases

There are some cases where the road users are likely to be exposed to risks. Some of them were:

Where motorcycle or bicycle slips on the rails,

Where wheelchair or bicycle wheels may get stuck in the rail flange gap, and Hazards for disabled people.

Therefore at the time of developing Countermeasures, the needs of people with disabilities and other vulnerable users should be particularly considered when developing countermeasures to ensure railway level crossing safety.
 

Factors of Level Crossing accidents

Whenever accidents occur at RLCs road users tends to blame the traffic control devices. Therefore the engineers need to consider the road user factors in order to plan and design control devices or making any improvements at RLCs. They should be aware of road user characteristics, capabilities, requirements, needs and obligation of users because they will help in designing proper method or better improvements at RLCs (Tustin et al., 1986). There are 3 main factors contributing to accidents at RLC in basic safety engineering studies. They are:

Human Factor

This is one of the major factors responsible for RLCs. This is mainly due to the driver's violation of rules.

Engineering Factor

This factor is of about the geometric design of the railway track making a clear vision of arriving train.

Environmental Factor

This factor deals with the environmental conditions prevailing at the RLCs in different seasons and their contribution to accidents.

Human Factor
 

Caird (2002) reported that many studies related with human factors contributing to accidents at RLC were conducted by many researchers mainly from Australia (Wigglesworth et al., 2001); Sweden (Aberg et al., 1988); Israel (Shinar et al., 1982) and the US (Klein et al., 1994; Lerner et al., 1990).
 

Familiarity of crossings is found to be the one of the main reason for accidents at RLCs. And on this, Wigglesworth (1978) conducted a case study of accidents occurred in Australia from 1973 to 1977 and he found that 87% of accidents were occurred due to the familiarity of the crossings.
 

Violation of rules by drivers is another reason for accidents. National Transport Safety Board (NTSB), US investigated in 1998 about 60 accident cases, out of which they found 49 cases were due to driver error. Of those 49 cases, 29 cases include driver's disregard for the stop sign and failure to look for a train. And the remaining cases are related to roadway and track conditions and affecting the ability of the driver to realize the passive crossing ahead and the attendance of approaching a train. Documented evidence (West Net Rail and Australia Western Railroad et al., July 2002) from train drivers indicates many situations where drivers ignore the signs or signals
 

Also risky behaviour is also one of the reasons for accidents. A case study was done by Witte in 2000 on 891 residents who are selected randomly in Michigan, and he found that 10 to 20 percent of them tried to beat the train which is considered a risky behaviour.
 

Slowing down of vehicles when approaching these RLCs is also another contributing factor for accidents (Moon et al., 2003, Ward et al., 1996). This occurs because of the misjudgement of the drivers whether to cross or not at passive crossings due to proper lack of vision.
 

Other factors such as long times may lead drivers to engage in riskier behaviour at crossings (Berg et al., 1982). This 'deliberate risk taking behaviour' results in major risks, particularly where heavy, long or slow vehicles are involved.
 

Research on Human Factor

This analysis was done by M W Pickett and G B Grayson, who are Researchers at Transportation Laboratory in Berkshire, UK.
 

Their study has examined a number of aspects of driver behaviour at level crossings.
 

A preliminary study was conducted by M W Pickett and G B Grayson in 1996. Their study was carried out on a sample of 419 witness statements which they obtained from the British Transport Police, North East Area. The majority of these statements were taken from drivers who had been observed by British Transport Police Officers violated activated warning systems at level crossings protected by automatic half barriers, and at open crossings.
 

The statements were analysed and classified according to whether the drivers claimed to have been unwilling to stop, unable to stop, or unaware of the crossing.
 

The results show that over a half of drivers (55%) were unwilling to stop at level crossings when the warning systems were activated. 13% of drivers were unable to stop, while just over one quarter (27%) claimed to be unaware of either the crossing or the lights, and 5% could not be classified. The reason given for doing this ranged from being late for work to simply not wanting to stop. Drivers that were apprehended for crossing while the lights were flashing red, and had claimed they crossed because someone was travelling too close behind, were also caused as unwilling.
 

Drivers who were on the crossing as the warning system came into operation were classed as being unable to stop. Similarly, those drivers who stated they had been followed too closely by other cars to be able to stop safely were classed as unable to stop.
 

Drivers were classified as being unaware of the crossing if they did not remember the incident, did not recall seeing the warning lights activated, or did not recall the crossing. Seven percent of violators stated they could not remember the incident.
 

Fifteen percent of the violators stated that they did not notice that the lights had been activated. One percent of violators reported that the position of the sun prevented them knowing that the lights were flashing.
 

This analysis has given some insight into why some drivers violate activated warning systems. However it is difficult to make generalisations about behaviour from this sample because of the possibility of misleading statements. It is also difficult to know from this data whether any changes to crossing design would raise driver's awareness, or whether improved driver education would reduce the number of offenders.
 

From the above data three categories of driver have been identified which are likely to be involved in accidents at level crossings.

those who are unwilling to stop,

those who are unable to stop, and

those who are unaware of the signals.
 



推荐内容
  • 英国作业
  • 新西兰作业
  • 爱尔兰作业
  • 美国作业
  • 加拿大作业
  • 英国essay
  • 澳洲essay
  • 美国essay
  • 加拿大essay
  • MBA Essay
  • Essay格式范文
  • 澳洲代写assignment
  • 代写英国assignment
  • 新西兰代写assignment
  • Assignment格式
  • 如何写assignment
  • 英国termpaper
  • 澳洲termpaper
  • 英国coursework代写
  • PEST分析法
  • literature review
  • Research Proposal
  • Reference格式
  • case study
  • presentation
  • report格式
  • Summary范文
  • common application
  • Personal Statement
  • Motivation Letter
  • Application Letter
  • recommendation letter